november 25, 2024

Koninkrijksrelaties

Dagelijks meer nieuwsberichten dan enige andere Nederlandse nieuwsbron over Nederland.

“Time Dilation” – Onze perceptie van tijd is vertraagd

“Time Dilation” – Onze perceptie van tijd is vertraagd

Hoe de pandemie en het sociale isolement onze kijk op het verstrijken van de tijd hebben veranderd.

In de vroege stadia van de pandemie zei de meerderheid van degenen die aan huis gebonden waren, dat ze het gevoel hadden dat de tijd langzamer ging en dat ze zich daardoor eenzaam voelden.

Volgens een bericht in het tijdschrift wetenschappelijke vooruitgang, De[{” attribute=””>COVID-19 pandemic has altered how individuals perceive the passing of time.

The majority of research participants (65%) reported feeling that time was moving more slowly at the conclusion of the first month of social isolation, which occurred in May 2020. This perception was termed by the researchers as “time expansion,” and they discovered that it was linked to feelings of isolation and a lack of enjoyable activities throughout the time period.

Even more people (75%) said they didn’t experience as much “time pressure,” which is the sensation that time is passing more quickly and leaving less time for activities of daily living and recreation. 90% of those surveyed claimed they were taking shelter at home during that time.

“We followed the volunteers for five months to see if this ‘snapshot’ of the start of the pandemic would change over time. We found that the feeling of time expansion diminished as the weeks went by, but we didn’t detect significant differences with regard to time pressure,” André Cravo, first author of the article, told Agência FAPESP. Cravo is a professor at the Federal University of ABC in São Paulo state, Brazil.

The research started on May 6, when 3,855 participants recruited via social media responded to a ten-item online questionnaire and completed a simple task meant to test their ability for short interval estimation (pressing start and stop buttons in 1, 3, and 12 seconds). They were then questioned about their daily activities the week before (including whether they had finished all required tasks and how much time they had set aside for leisure) as well as how they were feeling right now (happy, sad, lonely, etc).

“They were invited to return every week for further sessions, but not everyone did,” Cravo said. “In the final analysis, we considered data for 900 participants who answered the questionnaire for at least four weeks, albeit not all consecutively.”

Using time awareness scales from 0 to 100 that are standard for this type of survey, the researchers analyzed the answers and calculated the two parameters – time expansion and time pressure – to see whether they increased or decreased week by week.

“Besides a rise or fall on the scales, we also analyzed the factors that accompanied the changes. During the five-month period, we observed a similar pattern: in weeks when participants reported feeling lonely and experiencing less positive affect, they also felt time pass more slowly. In highly stressful situations, they felt time pass more quickly,” Cravo said.

When the first set of answers to the question on the passage of time was compared with the second, provided at the end of the first month of confinement, perceptions of time expansion had risen 20 points while time pressure had fallen 30 points, according to Raymundo Machado, a scientist at the Brain Institute of the Albert Einstein Jewish Hospital (HIAE) in São Paulo, and last author of the article. “These results are evidently affected by memory bias, however, because no measurements were made before the pandemic,” he said.

Time slowed most for younger participants early in the pandemic, when compliance with social distancing rules was strictest. Except for age, demographic factors such as household size, occupation, and gender, had no influence on the results.

For the authors, this may be an effect of the sample profile. Most of the volunteers (80.5%) lived in the Southeast region. A large majority were women (74.32%). Most had completed secondary school, and a great many even had a university degree (71.78%). In terms of income, roughly a third were upper middle class (33.08%). Sizable minorities worked in education (19.43%) and healthcare (15.36%).

“This is typical of online surveys, where a majority are women living in the Southeast with high levels of formal education. The influence of demographics might have been more evident if the sample had represented the Brazilian population better,” Machado said.

Internal clock

Although the pandemic changed participants’ perceptions of the passage of time, it apparently did not affect their ability to sense duration, measured by the button-pressing task. “All of us are able to estimate short intervals. When the results of this time estimation test [including overestimation and underestimation of the intervals] Ze werden vergeleken met scores voor tijdbewustzijn en er was geen correlatie”, zei Machado.

Volgens Cravo geeft bewijs uit de wetenschappelijke literatuur aan dat het gevoel dat de tijd langzamer of sneller verstrijkt voornamelijk wordt beïnvloed door twee factoren: het belang van tijd in een bepaalde context en de onvoorspelbaarheid ervan. Als je bijvoorbeeld te laat op je werk bent [so that time is relevant in the context] En ik moet wachten op de bus [unpredictable timing]Je hebt een extreme perceptie dat er geen minuten voorbij gaan. Als je op vakantie bent en plezier hebt, is het niet de tijd en lijkt het gewoon te vliegen.

Waarneming verandert vaak wanneer we ons situaties uit het verleden herinneren. “Als je je herinnert wat je tijdens je vakantie hebt gedaan, lijkt de tijd langer te duren. Integendeel, als je in de rij staat, gaat de tijd heel langzaam voorbij, maar als je je de situatie later herinnert, is het alsof het snel eindigde ’, zegt Cravo.

In het geval van de COVID-19-pandemie is het niet bekend hoe mensen zich het verstrijken van de tijd zullen herinneren tijdens de periode van sociale afstand. “Veel mijlpalen, zoals carnaval, juni-festivals en verjaardagen, moesten de afgelopen twee jaar worden overgeslagen, dus de vraag blijft open”, concludeerde hij.

Referentie: “Het ervaren van tijd tijdens sociale afstand: een longitudinaal onderzoek tijdens de eerste maanden van de COVID-19-pandemie in Brazilië” door André Macioli Cravo, Gustavo Brito de Azevedo, Cristiano Moraes Bellacci Azarias, Louise Catherine Barney, Fernanda Dantas Bueno en Rafael Manier. Di Camargo, Vanessa Carneiro Morita, Esau Ventura Bobo Sirius, Renan Schiavolin Recio, Mateus Silvestrin en Raimundo Machado de Azevedo Neto, 13 april 2022, wetenschap gaat vooruit.
DOI: 10.1126 / sciadv.abj7205

De studie werd gefinancierd door FAPESP.

READ  Dit is hoe Amerikaanse astronomen hielpen bij het ontdekken van een sterrenstelsel ‘op het niveau van Bigfoot’